Skip to content

In Enemy Garb: A New Explanation for Wasp Mimicry

wasp and moth mimics

Who is who? A stinging wasp (Polybia sp., middle) and two harmless day-active moths (both Pseudosphex laticincta) with near-perfect mimicry. Researchers in Germany and England have proposed that the moths evolved such mimicry to avoid not only predators such as birds or amphibians but also to avoid predation via the very wasps they imitate. (Photo credit: Hannes Freitag, Michael Boppré, Ph.D.)

By Michael Boppré, Ph.D.

A yellow-black insect at the coffee table—”Help, a wasp!” But, often, it is just an innocent hover fly, unable to sting. It is hard for us to separate these very similar looking insects.

In school we learn that many hover flies (and other insects) imitate wasps to be protected from predators (e.g., birds); these antagonists—so the textbook wisdom goes—learn from the painful experience attempting to prey upon stinging wasps, after which they avoid the wasps, as well as similar-looking hover flies. Such deception is quite common in the animal world and is called “Batesian mimicry,” after the naturalist who proposed the idea, Henry Walter Bates (1825‒1892).

Also, different species of wasps look alike: a bird (or reptile or amphibian) learns to recognise one of several similar-looking species as harmful, and all the others become equally protected from it. This is termed “Müllerian mimicry” in honour of Fritz Müller (1821‒1897). “Müllerian resemblance” would be the more correct term because, in contrast to Batesian mimicry, this is not a case of cheating but one of similarity (or “signal standardisation”) for mutual benefit. For wasps and their many mimics, both Batesian as well as Müllerian mimics often coexist, and a community of such species is called a “mimicry ring.”

Thus, for more than 150 years, “wasp mimicry” seems to have had a good explanation; even though in most cases the mimicry hypotheses have not yet been explicitly supported with empirical evidence, they are, at least, very plausible. Similarity to wasps (as with many other cases of Batesian mimicry) is often not really “perfect”—there are, in fact, more and less accurate mimics. In theory, this question also seems to be answered, as several additional hypotheses have been proposed to explain how “imperfect mimicry” can work well.

Studying biodiversity in the Neotropics, I encountered day-active moths (and other insects) that imitate wasps virtually perfectly: They have a wasp waist and a yellow-black pattern, their wings are transparent and even (as in wasps) folded, and their antennae appear just like wasp antennae. In particular, when flying, models and mimics are very hard to distinguish, even for trained eyes.

I asked myself how such “perfect” similarity can develop when imperfection seems sufficient for confusion. Who or what could generate selection that would lead to the evolution of greatest possible accuracy? My surprisingly simple answer: wasps!

Wasps are predators and they hunt insects as food for their larvae. Could the mimics, by looking just like wasps, protect themselves not only from learning birds and other vertebrate predators but also from attacks by their models, the wasps? By imitating their own enemies, could they be mixed up by the wasps, which innately do not attack their own nest-mates or conspecifics? Instead of signalling to birds that they are wasps (as assumed so far), it seems plausible that very precise similarity is sufficient to cause wasps, when foraging, to react to such mimics as if they were other wasps.

If this hypothesis is correct, then the similarity among many species of wasps can also be explained without invoking Müllerian mimicry: Wasps from their own nest (sisters) during hunting flights are not discriminated from wasps coming from other nests, or other similar-looking wasp species, and are thus not attacked. (From a human point of view, the wasps are, in a way, tricked, but wasps do not separate species but rather individuals, and the similar-looking individuals of other wasp species are equally unprofitable because of their stings.) Non-defended wasp mimics cheat wasps by looking like them and are likewise not attacked.

In the terminology of “adaptive resemblance”, the hypothesis belongs in the “masquerade” category. It receives support by certain black wasp species that are perfectly imitated by other (black) moths. Further, the probability that a wasp mimic meets a wasp is in many habitats probably much higher than being confronted by a learning vertebrate antagonist.

My co-authors—Dick Vane-Wright, Ph.D., of the Natural History Museum in London, and Wolfgang Wickler, Ph.D., of the Max-Planck-Institute for Ornithology in Seewiesen, Germany)—and I reported the new hypothesis in the journal Ecology and Evolution, where it is discussed at length and in a wider context. There we underline that the new hypothesis is not an alternative to the traditional explanations but rather an amendment or an addition; it will likely depend, within a given community and its qualitative and quantitative composition, how strong the selection effected by learning predators is in comparison to the selection brought about by wasps themselves.

Further studies are needed to find out if the doubts about the existence of wasp mimicry rings, which have been voiced as the result of various previous findings, are dispelled by the new hypothesis or not. Also, many gaps in knowledge are now apparent, and the need for new natural history studies is strongly underlined.

The new explanation at first glance might appear as a tiny detail, but the idea has wide-reaching consequences. The classical hypotheses, established for more than 150 years and directly relevant to Charles Darwin’s theory of evolution, go with additional assumptions—for instance that Batesian mimicry can only work if, at least temporarily, models outstrip mimics in number; only then the probability for learning by bad experience is given. The advantage of imitation (protection) goes with a disadvantage (lower abundance). Imitating wasps that innately do not attack mimics, however, would not appear to entail this limitation.

Ecology and Evolution

Read More

A hypothesis to explain accuracy of wasp resemblances

Ecology and Evolution

 

Michael Boppré, Ph.D., holds the chair of Forest Zoology and Entomology at the University of Freiburg in Freiburg im Breisgau, Germany. Email: boppre@fzi.uni-freiburg.de

2 Comments »

  1. How in the world is an insect supposed to changes its own colors? Can you change the color of your hair by wishing you weren’t so noticeable? Isn’t it more likely that the creator of all of the insects liked a certain pattern and used it on several different models?

    • Those insects don’t “change color”. Colors are encoded by genes, genes vary through recombination and mutation, offspring looking less appetizing gets eaten less by predators and eventually outnumbers their siblings. It’s a process taking numerous generations, but insect generations are more plentiful and populations larger than human populations, so changes happen quite faster in this mindless manner than they would for us, and we are talking about time spans outlasting human planning. But those processes don’t really keep up with the speed of human-caused habitat change (including the climate), so there is a whole lot of complete extinction going on rather than evolution, with “can better survive humans” being the dominant evolution-driving trait for almost all species.

Leave a Reply

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.